Associations between parental educational/occupational levels and cognitive performance in spanish adolescents: the avena study

  1. Castillo Gualda, Ruth
  2. Ruíz Ruíz, Jonatan
  3. Chillón Garzón, Palma
  4. Jiménez Pavón, David
  5. Díaz Prieto, Ligia Esperanza
  6. Moreno, Luis Antonio
  7. Ortega Porcel, Francisco Bartolomé
Revista:
Psicothema

ISSN: 0214-9915

Año de publicación: 2011

Volumen: 23

Número: 3

Páginas: 349-355

Tipo: Artículo

Otras publicaciones en: Psicothema

Resumen

Asociaciones entre el nivel educativo/profesional de los padres y rendimiento cognitivo en adolescentes españoles: el estudio AVENA. Se examinaron las asociaciones entre el nivel educativo/profesional de los padres y el rendimiento cognitivo de adolescentes españoles. El rendimiento cognitivo se midió mediante un test validado de inteligencia en 2.162 participantes. El nivel educativo y profesional de los padres se asoció positivamente con todas las habilidades cognitivas específi cas y la puntuación total (p<0,001 a 0,04). La probabilidad de que los adolescentes tuvieran un alto rendimiento cognitivo (cuartil superior) y padres con nivel educativo alto fue de 1,6 a 1,7 veces superior al de aquellos con padres de bajo nivel educativo. Del mismo modo, las probabilidades fueron de 1,9 a 2,4 veces mayor para los adolescentes con padres de nivel profesional alto. Estos resultados sugieren una asociación entre la educación/profesión de los padres y los niveles de rendimiento cognitivo en adolescentes y confi rman el papel que desempeñan los padres en la creación de un entorno intelectual estimulante.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Álvarez-Dardet, C., Alonso, J., Domingo, A., & Regidor, E. (1995). La medición de la clase social en Ciencias de la Salud. Informe de un grupo de trabajo de la Sociedad Española de Epidemiología [The measurement of social class in health sciences. Report of a working group of the Spanish Society of Epidemiology]. Barcelona: S G Editores.
  • Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., & Guajardo, S. (2005). The influence of the parents' educational level on the development of executive functions. Development Neuropsychology, 28(1), 539-560.
  • Batty, G.D., & Deary, I.J. (2005). Health communication, intelligence and health differentials. American Journal of Public Health, 95(7), 1088; author reply 1089.
  • Batty, G.D., Mortensen, E.L., Nybo Andersen, A.M., & Osler, M. (2005). Childhood intelligence in relation to adult coronary heart disease and stroke risk: Evidence from a Danish birth cohort study. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 19(6), 452-459.
  • Batty, G.D., Mortensen, E.L., & Osler, M. (2005). Childhood IQ in relation to later psychiatric disorder: Evidence from a Danish birth cohort study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 187, 180-181.
  • Berra, S., Borrell, C., Rajmil, L., Estrada, M.D., Rodríguez, M., Riley, A. W., et al. (2006). Perceived health status and use of healthcare services among children and adolescents. European Journal of Public Health, 16(4), 405-414.
  • Bradley, R.H., & Corwyn, R.F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 371-399.
  • Brissos, S., Dias, V., & Kapczinski, F. (2008). Cognitive performance and quality of life in bipolar disorder. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 53(8) 517-524.
  • Chillón, P., Ortega, F.B., Ruiz, J.R., Pérez, I.J., Martín-Matillas, M., Valtuena, J., et al. (2009). Socio-economic factors and active commuting to school in urban Spanish adolescents: The AVENA study. European Journal of Public Health, 19(5), 470-476.
  • Dombrovski, A., Butters, M., Reynolds, C., Houck, P., Clark, L., Mazumdar, S., et al. (2008). Cognitive performance in suicidal depressed elderly: Preliminary report. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16(2) 109-115.
  • Eaton, W. W., Muntaner, C., Bovasso, G., & Smith, C. (2001). Socioeconomic status and depressive syndrome: The role of inter- and intra-generational mobility, government assistance and work environment. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42(3), 277-294.
  • Fati-Ashtiani, A., Ejei, J., Khodapanahi, M., & Tarkhorani, H. (2007). Relation between Self-concept, Self-steem, Anxiety, Depression and Acedemic achievement in adolescents. Journal of Applied Sciences, 7(7), 995-1000.
  • González-Gross, M., Castillo, M.J., Moreno, L., Nova, E., González-Lamuno, D., Pérez-Llamas, F., et al. (2003). [Feeding and assessment of nutritional status of spanish adolescents (AVENA study). Evaluation of risks and interventional proposal. I. Methodology]. Nutrición Hospitalaria, 18(1), 15-28.
  • Gottfredson, L.S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence, 24, 79-132.
  • Hackman, D.A., & Farah, M.J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(2), 65-73.
  • Maldonado, E., Fernández, F., Trianes, M.V., Wesnes, K., Petrini, O., Zangara, A., et al. (2008). Cognitive performance and morning levels of salivary cortisol and alpha -amylase in children reporting high vs low daily stress perception. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 11(1), 3-15.
  • Martin, L.T., Fitzmaurice, G.M., Kindlon, D.J., & Buka, S.L. (2004). Cognitive performance in childhood and early adult illness: Aprospective cohort study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 58(8), 674-679.
  • Martin, L.T., & Kubzansky, L.D. (2005). Childhood cognitive performance and risk of mortality: A prospective cohort study of gifted individuals. American Journal of Epidemiology, 162(9), 887-890.
  • Martin, L.T., Kubzansky, L.D., LeWinn, K.Z., Lipsitt, L.P., Satz, P., & Buka, S.L. (2007). Childhood cognitive performance and risk of generalized anxiety disorder. International Journal of Epidemiology, 36(4), 769-775.
  • Melchior, M., Moffitt, T.E., Milne, B.J., Poulton, R., & Caspi, A. (2007). Why do children from socioeconomically disadvantaged families suffer from poor health when they reach adulthood? A life-course study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 166(8), 966-974.
  • Merikangas, K.R. (2005). Vulnerability factors for anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. Child and Adolescents Psychiatric Clinic of North America, 14(4), 649-679, VII.
  • Molfese, V., Dilalla, L., & Bunce, D. (1997). Prediction of the Intelligence Test Scores of 3 to 8 year old children by home environment, socioeconomic status and biomedical risks. Proquest Psychology Journal, 43(2), 219-234.
  • Moreno, L. A., Fleta, J., Mur, L., Feja, C., Sarria, A., & Bueno, M. (1997). Indices of body fat distribution in Spanish children aged 4.0 to 14.9 years. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 25(2), 175-181.
  • Moreno, L.A., Joyanes, M., Mesana, M.I., González-Gross, M., Gil, C.M., Sarria, A., et al. (2003). Harmonization of anthropometric measurements for a multicenter nutrition survey in Spanish adolescents. Nutrition, 19(6), 481-486.
  • Nakagawa, S., & Cuthill, I.C. (2007). Effect size, confidence interval and statistical significance: A practical guide for biologists. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 82(4), 591-605.
  • Noble, K.G., Farah, M.J., & McCandliss, B.D. (2006). Socioeconomic background modulates cognition-achievement relationships in reading. Cognitive Development, 21(3), 349-368.
  • Noble, K.G., McCandliss, B.D., & Farah, M.J. (2007). Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Development Science, 10(4), 464-480.
  • Schmidt, F., & Hunter, J. (2004). General mental ability in the world of work: Occupational attainment and job performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 162-173.
  • Sirin, S. (2005). Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A metaanalytic review of research. Review of Educational Research, 75(3), 417-453.
  • Strenze, T. (2007). Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A metanalytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence, 35, 401-426.
  • Sutton, A., & Soderstrom, I. (1999). Predicting elementary and secondary school achievement with school-related and demographic factors. Journal of Educational Research, 92(6), 330-338.
  • Tanner, J.M., & Whitehouse, R.H. (1976). Clinical longitudinal standards for height, weight, height velocity and stages of puberty. Archive of Disease in Childhood, 51, 170-179.
  • Thurstone, L., & Thurstone, T. (1958). SRA Test of Educational Ability. Chicago: Science Research Associates.
  • Thurstone, L., & Thurstone, T. (1998). TEA Test de Aptitudes Escolares (Scholar Aptitudes Test). Madrid: Sección de Estudio de Test de Técnicos Especialistas Asociados.
  • Tong, S., Baghurst, P., Vimpani, G., & McMichael, A. (2007). Socioeconomic position, maternal IQ, home environment, and cognitive development. Journal of Pediatric, 151(3), 284-288.
  • Villaseñor, E., Martín, A., Díaz, E., Rosselli, M., & Ardila, A. (2009). Effects of parents' educational level, school type and gender on the development of attention and memory. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 41(2), 257-276.
  • Whalley, L.J., & Deary, I.J. (2001). Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and survival up to age 76. British Medical Journal, 322, 819-822.
  • Yarnell, J., Yu, S., McCrum, E., Arveiler, D., Hass, B., Dallongeville, J., et al. (2005). Education, socioeconomic and lifestyle factors and risk of coronary heart disease: The PRIME Study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 34(2), 268-275.