Modifying Evaluations and Decisions in Risky Situations

  1. Antonio Maldonado 1
  2. Sara Serra 1
  3. Andrés Catena 1
  4. Antonio Cándido 1
  5. Alberto Megías 1
  1. 1 Universidad de Granada (Spain)
Revista:
The Spanish Journal of Psychology

ISSN: 1138-7416

Año de publicación: 2016

Volumen: 19

Tipo: Artículo

DOI: 10.1017/SJP.2016.51 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso abierto editor

Otras publicaciones en: The Spanish Journal of Psychology

Resumen

The main aim of this research was to investigate the decision making process in risky situations. We studied how different types of feedback on risky driving behaviors modulate risk evaluation and risk-taking. For a set of risky traffic situations, participants had to make evaluative judgments (judge the situation as risky or not) and urgent decisions (brake or not). In Experiment 1, participants received feedback with and without negative emotional content when they made risky behaviors. In Experiment 2 we investigated the independent effects of feedback and negative emotional stimuli. The results showed three important findings: First, urgent decisions were faster [F(1, 92) = 6.76, p = .01] and more cautious [F(1, 92) = 17.16, p < .001] than evaluative judgments. These results suggest that evaluative judgments of risk and actual risk-taking may not always coincide, and that they seem to be controlled by two different processing systems as proposed by dual process theories. Second, feedback made participants’ responses even faster [F(1, 111) = 71.53, p < .001], allowing greater risk sensitivity [F(1, 111) = 22.12, p < .001] and skewing towards more cautious responses [F(1, 111) = 14.09, p < .001]. Finally, emotional stimuli had an effect only when they were presented as feedback. The results of this research increase our understanding of the processes involved in risky driving behavior and suggest efficient ways to control risk taking through the use of feedback.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Alos-Ferrer C., & Strack F. (2014). From dual processes to multiple selves: Implications for economic behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 41, 1–11. http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/j.joep.2013.12.005
  • Bower G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 0003-066X.36.2.129
  • Contreras D., Catena A., Cándido A., Perales J. C., & Maldonado A. (2008). Funciones de la corteza prefrontal ventromedial en la toma de decisiones emocionales [Function of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotional decision making]. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 8(1), 285–313.
  • Contreras D., Megías A., Maldonado A., Cándido A., & Catena A. (2013). Facilitation and interference of behavioral responses by task-irrelevant affect-laden stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 37, 496–507. http://dx. doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9327-0
  • Deery H. A. (1999). Hazard and risk perception among young novice drivers. Journal of Safety Research, 30, 225–236. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4375(99)00018-3
  • Di Stasi L. L., Álvarez-Valbuena V., Cañas J. J., Maldonado A., Catena A., Antolí A., & Cándido A. (2009). Risk behavior and mental workload: Multimodal assessment techniques applied to motorbike riding simulation. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 12, 361–370. http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/j.trf.2009.02.004
  • Di Stasi L. L., Contreras D., Cañas J. J., Cándido A., Maldonado A., & Catena A. (2010). The consequences of unexpected emotional sounds on driving behavior in risky situations. Safety Science, 48, 1463–1468. http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/j.ssci.2010.07.006
  • Donmez B., Boyle L. N., & Lee J. D. (2007). Safety implications of providing real-time feedback to distracted drivers. Accident, Analysis and Prevention, 39, 581–590. http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/j.aap.2006.10.003
  • Edman G., Schalling D., & Levander S. E. (1983). Impulsivity and speed and errors in a reaction time task: A contribution to the construct validity of the concept of impulsivity. Acta Psychologica, 53(1), 1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(83)90012-4
  • Evans J. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/ annurev.psych.59.103006.093629
  • Forgas J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 39–66. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.39
  • Gregersen N. P., & Bjurulf P. (1996). Young novice drivers: Towards a model of their accident involvement. Accident, Analysis and Prevention, 28, 229–241. http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/0001-4575(95)00063-1
  • Groeger J. A. (2000). Understanding driving: Applying cognitive psychology to a complex everyday task. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • Kahneman D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
  • Kinnear N., Kelly S. W., Stradling S., & Thomsonb J. (2013). Understanding how drivers learn to anticipate risk on the road: A laboratory experiment of affective anticipation of road hazards. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 50, 1025–1033. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.08.008
  • Lerner J. S., & Keltner D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 146–159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.146
  • Lieberman M. (2003). Reflexive and reflective judgment processes: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. In J. P. Forgas, K. D. Williams, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), Social judgments: Explicit and implicit processes (pp. 44–67). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Loewenstein G. F., Weber E. U., Hsee C. K., & Welch N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267–286. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.267
  • Macmillan N. A., & Creelman C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide. (2nd Ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Megías A. (2014). Modulación emocional del comportamiento urgente y evaluativo en situaciones de riesgo en conducción [Emotional modulation of urgent and evaluative behavior in risky driving situations]. (Published PhD Thesis). Granada, Spain: Universidad de Granada.
  • Megías A., Di Stasi L., Maldonado A., Catena A., & Cándido A. (2014). Emotion-laden stimuli influence our reactions to traffic lights. Transportation Research Part F, 22, 96–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2013.09.017
  • Megías A., López-Riañez M., & Cándido A. (2013). Conductas urgentes y evaluativas en función del nivel de riesgo en situaciones de conducción [Urgent and evaluative behaviours according to the risk level in driving situations]. Anales de Psicología, 29, 1032–1037. http://dx.doi. org/10.6018/analesps.29.3.145451
  • Megías A., Maldonado A., Cándido A., & Catena A. (2011). Emotional modulation of urgent and evaluative behaviors in risky driving scenarios. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 43, 813–817. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. aap.2010.10.029
  • Megías A., Maldonado A., Catena A., Di Stasi L., Serrano J., & Cándido A. (2011). Modulation of attention and urgent decisions by affect-laden roadside advertisement in risky driving scenarios. Safety Science, 49, 1388–1393. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2011.06.001
  • Megías A., Navas J. F., Petrova D., Cándido A., Maldonado A., Garcia-Retamero R., & Catena A. (2015). Neural mechanisms underlying urgent and evaluative behaviors: An fMRI study on the interaction of automatic and controlled processes. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 2853–2864. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ hbm.22812
  • Mesken J. (2006). Determinants and consequences of drivers’emotions. Leidschendam, The Netherlands: Institute for Road Safety Research (SWOV).
  • Nazif J. I., & Perez G. (2011). Experiencias internacionales en campañas integrales y efectivas de seguridad vial [International experiences in comprehensive and effective road safety campaigns]. Boletín FAL. United Nations ECLAC, 294.
  • Nicholson N., Soane E., Fenton-O’Creevy M., & Willman P. (2005). Personality and domain specific risk taking. Journal of Risk Research, 8, 157–176. http://dx.doi. org/10.1080/1366987032000123856
  • Niv Y. (2007). Cost, benefit, tonic, phasic. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1104(1), 357–376. http://dx.doi. org/10.1196/annals.1390.018
  • Pêcher C., Lemercier C., & Cellier J. M. (2009). Emotions drive attention: Effects on driver’s behavior. Safety Science, 47, 1254–1259. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2009.03.011
  • Pessoa L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 160–166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.006
  • Rangel A., Camerer C., & Montague P. R. (2008). A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 545–556.
  • Reyna V. F. (2004). How people make decisions that involve risk. A dual-processes approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 60–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ j.0963-7214.2004.00275.x
  • Rhodes N., & Pivik K. (2011). Age and gender differences in risky driving: The roles of positive affect and risk perception. Accident, Analysis and Prevention, 43, 923–931. http://dx. doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2010.11.015
  • Serrano J., Di Stasi L. L., Megías A., & Catena A. (2014). Affective sound effects on driving behavior. Transport, 29(1), 100–106. http://dx.doi.org/10.3846/16484142.2013.815133
  • Simen P., Contreras D., Buck C., Hu P., Holmes P., & Cohen J. D. (2009). Reward rate optimization in two-alternative decision making: Empirical tests of theoretical predictions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1865–1897. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016926
  • Simen P., Cohen J. D., & Holmes P. (2006). Rapid decision threshold modulation by reward rate in a neural network. Neural Networks, 19, 1013–1026. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. neunet.2006.05.038
  • Slovic P., Finucane M. L., Peters E., & MacGregor D. G. (2007). The affect heuristic. European Journal of Operational Research, 177, 1333–1352. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. ejor.2005.04.006
  • Strack F., & Deutsch R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247. http://dx.doi. org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1
  • Vila J., Sánchez M., Ramírez I., Fernández M. C., Cobos P., Rodríguez S., & Moltó J. (2001). The International affective picture system (IAPS): Spanish adaptation. Second part. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 54, 635–657.
  • World Health Organization (2013). Global status report on road safety 2013: Supporting a decade of action. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
  • World Medical Association. (2013). World medical association declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. Jama, 310, 2191.