Proportion Congruent effects in the absence of Sequential Congruent effects

  1. Maryem Torres-Quesada 1
  2. Bruce Milliken 2
  3. Juan Lupiáñez 1
  4. María Jesús Funes 1
  1. 1 University of Granada, Spain
  2. 2 McMaster University, Canada
Revista:
Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

ISSN: 1576-8597

Año de publicación: 2014

Volumen: 35

Número: 1

Páginas: 101-115

Tipo: Artículo

Otras publicaciones en: Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

Resumen

En el campo del control cognitivo, una pregunta de gran interés es si el control cognitivo está formado por uno o varios mecanismos. Una forma de responder a esta pregunta ha sido la disociación de dos efectos relacionados con control cognitivo: los efectos secuenciales y los efectos de proporción de congruencia. De forma similar, este experimento tiene como objetivo disociar ambos efectos pero en este caso investigando si los efectos de proporción de congruencia se producen en ausencia de los efectos secuenciales. Para ello se presentaron dos tipos de conflicto mezclados aleatoriamente (Simon y Stroop Espacial) y con la proporción de congruencia manipulada en uno de ellos, manteniendo neutral el número de ensayos congruentes e incongruentes en el otro conflicto. Nuestros resultados mostraron que en los ensayos en los que se producía una alternancia de tipo de conflicto, y dónde los efectos secuenciales estaban ausentes, se observaron efectos de proporción de congruencia. Esto indica que, al menos en circunstancias concretas, los efectos de proporción de congruencia y secuenciales son independientes y específicos al tipo de conflicto.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Blais, C., Robidoux, S., Risko, E. F., & Besner, D. (2007). Item specific adaptation and the conflict monitoring hypothesis: A computational model. Psychological Review, 114, 1076-1086
  • Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624-652
  • Botvinick, M. M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K. L., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulated cortex. Nature, 402, 179-181
  • Braver, T. S., Gray, J. R., & Burgess, G. C. (2007). Explaining the may varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. In A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake & J. Towse (Eds.), Variation in Working Memory (pp. 76-106). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Bugg, J. M., & Crump, M. J. C. (2012). In support of a distinction between voluntary and stimulus-driven control: A review of the literature on proportion congruent effects. Frontiers in Cognition, 3
  • Cañadas, E., Rodríguez-Bailón, R., Milliken, B., & Lupiáñez, J. (in press). Social Categories as a Context for the Allocation of Attentional Control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, No pagination specified.
  • Carter, C. S., MacDonald, A. M., Botvinick, M. M., Ross, L. L., Stenger, V. A., Noll, D., et al. (2000). Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulated cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science,USA, 97, 1944-1948
  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing model of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332-361
  • Crump, M. J. C., Gong, Z., & Milliken, B. (2006). The context-specific proportion of congruent Stroop effect: Location as contextual cue. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(2), 121-125
  • Crump, M. J. C., Vaquero, J. M., & Milliken, B. (2008). Context-specific learning and control: the roles of awareness, and relative salience. Consciousness & Cognition, 17, 22-36
  • DePisapia, N., & Braver, T. S. (2006). A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions. Neurocomputing, 69, 1322-1326
  • Egner, T., Delano, M., & Hirsch, J. (2007). Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain. NeuroImage, 35, 940-948
  • Funes, M. J., Lupiáñez, J., & Humphreys, G. (2010a). Analyzing the generality of conflict adaptation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Psychophysics, 36(1), 147-161
  • Funes, M. J., Lupiáñez, J., & Humphreys, G. (2010b). Sustained versus transient cognitive control: evidence of a behavioral dissociation. Cognition, 114(3), 338-347
  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information- Strategic control activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology- General, 121, 480-506
  • Jacoby, L. L., Lindsay, D. S., & Hessels, S. (2003). Item-specific control of automatic process: Stroop process dissociations. Psychological Bulletin & Review, 10, 638-644
  • Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. M., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control Science, 303, 1023-1026
  • Kunde, W., & Wühr, P. (2006). Sequential modulations of correspondence effects across spatial dimensions and tasks. Memory and Cognition, 34, 356-367
  • Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, N. J. (1979). When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task. Memory and Cognition, 7, 166-174
  • Lowe, D., & Mitterer, J. O. (1982). Selective and divided attention in a Stroop task. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 684-700
  • Macleod, C. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163-203
  • Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2008). Cognitive control acts locally. Cognition, 106, 1071- 1080
  • Riggio, L., Gherri, E., & Lupiáñez, J. (2012). Onset and offset as determinants of the Simon effect. Psicológica, 33(2), 209-236
  • Schmidt, J. R. (in press). Questioning conflict adaptation: proportion congruent and gratton effects reconsidered. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (Eds.). (2002a). E-Prime Useŕs Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (Eds.). (2002b). E-Prime Reference Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
  • Torres-Quesada, M., Funes, M. J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2013). Dissociating Proportion Congruent and Conflict Adaptation effects in a Simon-Stroop procedure. Acta Psychologica, 142
  • Torres-Quesada, M., Lupiáñez, J., Milliken, B., & Funes, M. J. (submitted). Proportion Congruent effects in the absence of Sequential Congruent effects: Analyzing their properties.
  • Verguts, T., & Notebaert, W. (2008). Hebbian learning of cognitive control: dealing with specific and nonspecific adaptation. Psychological Review, 115(2), 518-525
  • Wendt, M., Kluwe, R. H., & Peters, A. (2006). Sequential modulation of interference evoked by processing task-irrelevant stimulus features Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 32, 644-667
  • West, R., & Baylis, G. C. (1998). Effects of increased response dominance and contextual disintegration on the Stroop interference effect in older adults. Consciousness & Cognition, 17, 206-217.