The ANTI-Vea taskAnalyzing the executive and arousal vigilance decrements while measuring the three attentional networks

  1. Fernando G. Luna 1
  2. Pablo Barttfeld 1
  3. Elisa Martín-Arévalo 2
  4. Juan Lupiáñez 2
  1. 1 Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Córdoba, Argentina
  2. 2 University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Revista:
Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

ISSN: 1576-8597

Año de publicación: 2021

Volumen: 42

Número: 1

Páginas: 1-26

Tipo: Artículo

DOI: 10.2478/PSICOLJ-2021-0001 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso abierto editor

Otras publicaciones en: Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

Resumen

The vigilance decrement phenomenon has been traditionally studied by simple and monotonous behavioral tasks. Nowadays, however, there is considerable interest in measuring vigilance with more complex tasks, including independent measures of other attentional functions. In the present study, we provide evidence supporting the suitability of the Attentional Networks Test for Interactions and Vigilance –executive and arousal components (ANTI-Vea) as an appropriate method to simultaneously assess multiple attentional and vigilance components. Vigilance was examined as two dissociated components: executive vigilance –as the detection of infrequent signals–and arousal vigilance –as the sustenance of a fast reaction to stimuli without response selection–. Importantly, the executive vigilance decrement was analyzed with a novel methodological approach to particularly determine whether the sensitivity loss effect is influenced by a floor level on the false alarms. As expected, the ANTI-Vea proved to be a task suitable to assess: (a) the main effects and interactions of phasic alertness, orienting, and executive control; (b) the executive vigilance decrement as a progressive change in the response bias; and (c) the arousal vigilance decrement as a progressive slownessand variability in reaction time. We discuss some critical theoretical and empirical implications of measuring vigilance components with the ANTI-Vea task. We expect the present study to provide a suitable method to analyze the vigilance decrement phenomenon when measuring multiple attentional and vigilance functions.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Adams, J. A. (1987). Criticisms of Vigilance Research: A Discussion. Human Factors, 29(6), 737–740. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872088702900612
  • Basner, M., & Dinges, D. F. (2011). Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep, 34(5), 581–591. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/34.5.581
  • Bastien, C. H., Vallières, A., & Morin, C. M. (2001). Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Medicine, 2(4), 297–307. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00065-4
  • Broadbent, D. E., Cooper, P. F., FitzGerald, P., & Parkes, K. R. (1982). The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21(1), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1982.tb01421.x
  • Callejas, A., Lupiáñez, J., Funes, M. J., & Tudela, P. (2005). Modulations among the alerting, orienting and executive control networks. Experimental Brain Research, 167(1), 27–37. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-2365-z
  • Callejas, A., Lupiáñez, J., & Tudela, P. (2004). The three attentional networks: on their independence and interactions. Brain and Cognition, 54(3), 225–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.012
  • Claypoole, V. L., Neigel, A. R., Fraulini, N. W., Hancock, G. M., & Szalma, J. L. (2018). Can vigilance tasks be administered online? A replication and discussion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(9), 1348–1355. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000538
  • Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1(1), 42–45. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.01.1.p042
  • Cumming, G. (2014). The New Statistics: Why and How. Psychological Science, 25(1), 7–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613504966
  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2), 225–236. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.225
  • Drummond, S. P. a, Bischoff-Grethe, A., Dinges, D. F., Ayalon, L., Mednick, S. C., & Meloy, M. J. (2005). The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task. Sleep, 28(9), 1059–1068. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/28.9.1059
  • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–347. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361886
  • Fortenbaugh, F. C., DeGutis, J., & Esterman, M. (2017). Recent theoretical, neural, and clinical advances in sustained attention research. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1396(1), 70–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13318
  • Fraulini, N. W., Hancock, G. M., Neigel, A. R., Claypoole, V. L., & Szalma, J. L. (2017). A Critical Examination of the Research and Theoretical Underpinnings Discussed in Thomson, Besner, and Smilek (2016). Psychological Review, 124(4), 525–531. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000066
  • Grier, J. B. (1971). Nonparametric indexes for sensitivity and bias: computing formulas. Psychological Bulletin, 75(6), 424–429. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031246
  • Grier, R. A., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N., Matthews, G., Galinsky, T. L., Szalma, J. L., & Parasuraman, R. (2003). The Vigilance Decrement Reflects Limitations in Effortful Attention, Not Mindlessness. Human Factors, 45(3), 349–359. https://doi.org/10.1518/hfes.45.3.349.27253
  • Hancock, P. A. (2017). On the Nature of Vigilance. Human Factors, 59(1), 35–43. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720816655240
  • Huertas, F., Ballester, R., Gines, H. J., Hamidi, A. K., Moratal, C., & Lupiáñez, J. (2019). Relative Age Effect in the Sport Environment. Role of Physical Fitness and Cognitive Function in Youth Soccer Players. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(16), 2837. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16162837
  • Hunter, J. D. (2007). Matplotlib: A 2D graphics enviroment. Computing In Science & Engineering, 9(3), 90–95. https://doi.org/10.1109/MCSE.2007.55
  • JASP Team. (2019). JASP (Version 0.11.1). Retrieved from https://jasp-stats.org/
  • Jun, J., Remington, R. W., Koutstaal, W., & Jiang, Y. V. (2019). Characteristics of sustaining attention in a gradual-onset continuous performance task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(3), 386–401. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000604
  • Kelley, K., & Preacher, K. J. (2012). On effect size. Psychological Methods, 17(2), 137–152. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028086
  • Lamond, N., Dawson, D., & Roach, G. D. (2005). Fatigue assessment in the field: Validation of a hand-held electronic psychomotor vigilance task. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 76(5), 486–489.
  • Lamond, N., Jay, S. M., Dorrian, J., Ferguson, S. a, Roach, G. D., & Dawson, D. (2008). The sensitivity of a palm-based psychomotor vigilance task to severe sleep loss. Behavior Research Methods, 40(1), 347–352. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.1.347
  • Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2013). Sustaining attention to simple tasks: A meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 870–900. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030694
  • Lim, J., & Dinges, D. F. (2008). Sleep Deprivation and Vigilant Attention. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1129(1), 305–322. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1417.002
  • Loh, S., Lamond, N., Dorrian, J., Roach, G., & Dawson, D. (2004). The validity of psychomotor vigilance tasks of less than 10-minute duration. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(2), 339–346. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195580
  • Luna, F. G., Marino, J., Roca, J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2018). Executive and arousal vigilance decrement in the context of the attentional networks: The ANTI-Vea task. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 306, 77–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.05.011
  • Luna, F. G., Roca, J., Martín-Arévalo, E., & Lupiáñez, J. (2020). Measuring attention and vigilance in the laboratory vs. online: The split-half reliability of the ANTI-Vea. Behavior Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01483-4
  • Luna, F. G., Román-Caballero, R., Barttfeld, P., Lupiáñez, J., & Martín-Arévalo, E. (2020). A High-Definition tDCS and EEG study on attention and vigilance: Brain stimulation mitigates the executive but not the arousal vigilance decrement. Neuropsychologia, 142, 107447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107447
  • Luna, F. G., Telga, M., Vadillo, M. A., & Lupiáñez, J. (2020). Concurrent working memory load may increase or reduce cognitive interference depending on the attentional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 46(7), 667–680. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000740
  • Mackworth, N. H. (1948). The Breakdown of Vigilance during Prolonged Visual Search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1(1), 6–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470214808416738
  • Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(6), 768–774. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AIDJCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1
  • Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The Attention System of the Human Brain: 20 Years After. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35(1), 73–89. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525
  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The Attention System of The Human Brain. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325
  • Psychology Software Tools, Inc. (2012). [E-Prime 2.0]. Retrieved from http://www.pstnet.com
  • Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). “Oops!”: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), 747–758. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00015-8
  • Roca, J., Castro, C., López-Ramón, M. F., & Lupiáñez, J. (2011). Measuring vigilance while assessing the functioning of the three attentional networks: the ANTI-Vigilance task. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 198(2), 312–324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.04.014
  • Roca, J., García-Fernández, P., Castro, C., & Lupiáñez, J. (2018). The moderating effects of vigilance on other components of attentional functioning. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 308, 151–161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.07.019
  • Román-Caballero, R., Martín-Arévalo, E., & Lupiáñez, J. (2020). Attentional networks functioning and vigilance in expert musicians and non-musicians. Psychological Research, (0123456789). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01323-2
  • Sanchis, C., Blasco, E., Luna, F. G., & Lupiáñez, J. (2020). Effects of caffeine intake and exercise intensity on executive and arousal vigilance. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 8393. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-65197-5
  • Scerbo, M. W. (1998). 50 Years of Vigilance Research: Where are we now? Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 42(22), 1575–1579. https://doi.org/10.1177/154193129804202209
  • See, J. E., Howe, S. R., Warm, J. S., & Dember, W. N. (1995). Meta-analysis of the sensitivity decrement in vigilance. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2), 230–249. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.230
  • Stanislaw, H., & Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31(1), 137–149. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03207704
  • Thomson, D. R., Besner, D., & Smilek, D. (2015). A resource-control account of sustained attention: Evidence from mind-wandering and vigilance paradigms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(1), 82–96. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614556681
  • Thomson, D. R., Besner, D., & Smilek, D. (2016). A critical examination of the evidence for sensitivity loss in modern vigilance tasks. Psychological Review, 123(1), 70–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000021
  • Tkachenko, O., & Dinges, D. F. (2018). Interindividual variability in neurobehavioral response to sleep loss: A comprehensive review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 89(October 2017), 29–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.03.017
  • Wagenmakers, E.-J., Love, J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., … Morey, R. D. (2018). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 58–76. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1323-7
  • Warm, J. S., Parasuraman, R., & Matthews, G. (2008). Vigilance Requires Hard Mental Work and Is Stressful. Human Factors, 50(3), 433–441. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872008X312152
  • Wickens, C. D., Hutchins, S. D., Laux, L., & Sebok, A. (2015). The Impact of Sleep Disruption on Complex Cognitive Tasks. Human Factors, 57(6), 930–946. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720815571935
  • Zholdassova, M., Kustubayeva, A., & Matthews, G. (2019). The ANT Executive Control Index: No Evidence for Temporal Decrement. Human Factors, 001872081988005. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720819880058