El papel de las habilidades de ritmo no-lingüístico en las primeras etapas del aprendizaje formal de la lectura

  1. Nieves Valencia-Naranjo
  2. Nuria Calet
  3. María Auxiliadora Robles-Bello
  4. Nicolás Gutiérrez-Palma
Revista:
Revista de psicodidáctica

ISSN: 1136-1034

Any de publicació: 2024

Volum: 29

Número: 2

Pàgines: 166-175

Tipus: Article

DOI: 10.1016/J.PSICOD.2024.04.001 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR

Altres publicacions en: Revista de psicodidáctica

Resum

El estudio de los factores que contribuyen al aprendizaje de la lectoescritura incluye factores de tipo prosódico. La evidencia sugiere que las habilidades rítmicas de las niñas y niños participan en este proceso de adquisición, aunque algunos resultados son inconsistentes. El primer objetivo de este trabajo consiste en determinar si las habilidades rítmicas no-lingüísticas predicen la lectura una vez controlados el vocabulario, la inteligencia y la conciencia fonológica/conciencia del acento. El segundo objetivo consiste en comprobar si la conciencia fonológica o del acento media la relación entre ritmo no-lingüístico y lectura, tal y como cabe esperar según la teoría del muestreo temporal de Goswami. Los participantes de este estudio son niñas y niños españoles que han iniciado el aprendizaje formal de la lectura (primer curso de Educación Primaria). Estos niños y niñas deben realizar diversas tareas fonológicas, rítmicas, y de lectura. En relación con el primer objetivo, los resultados han sugerido que la capacidad predictiva del ritmo depende del tipo de ritmo no-lingüístico, y que el ritmo más lento (1.5 Hz) ha contribuido de forma independiente a la lectura de palabras (reales e inventadas) una vez se controla la conciencia fonológica. En relación con el segundo objetivo, los resultados han mostrado una relación indirecta entre el ritmo más rápido (2 Hz) y la lectura de pseudopalabras mediada por la conciencia fonológica. Además, la conciencia del acento media la relación entre el ritmo más lento y la lectura de pseudopalabras. Estos resultados han puesto de manifiesto el papel del ritmo en el aprendizaje de la lectura.

Referències bibliogràfiques

  • [Arvaniti, 2009] Rhythm, timing and the timing of rhythm. Phonetica, 66 (2009), pp. 46-63 https://doi.org/10.1159/000208930
  • [Calet et al., 2016] Habilidades fonológicas suprasegmentales y desarrollo lector en niños de educación primaria. Anales de Psicología, 32 (2016), pp. 72-79 https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.32.1.216221
  • [Calet et al., 2015] Suprasegmental phonology development and reading acquisition: A longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 19 (2015), pp. 51-71 https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2014.976342
  • [Corriveau et al., 2010] Auditory processing and early literacy skills in a preschool and kindergarten population. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43 (2010), pp. 369-382 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219410369071 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219410369071
  • [Cuetos et al., 2012] PROLEC-R. Batería de Evaluación de los Procesos Lectores -Revisada. TEA, (2012)
  • [David et al., 2007] Rhythm and reading development in school-age children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Reading, 30 (2007), pp. 169-183 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00323.x
  • [Defior, 1996] Una clasificación de las tareas utilizadas en la evaluación de las habilidades fonológicas y algunas ideas para su mejora. Journal for the Study of Education and Development. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 73 (1996), pp. 49-64
  • [Defior et al., 2012] Prosodic awareness skills and literacy acquisition in Spanish. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 41 (2012), pp. 285-294 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10936-011-9192-0 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-011-9192-0
  • [Defior et al., 2006] Habilidades de análisis y síntesis fonémica: Su evolución y relación con la lectoescritura. pp. 323-333 https://dialnet.unirioja.es/servlet/libro?codigo=525209
  • [Dellatolas et al., 2009] Rhythm reproduction in kindergarten, reading performance at second grade, and developmental dyslexia theories. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24 (2009), pp. 555-563 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acp044 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acp044
  • [Fawcett y Nicolson, 2004] The Dyslexia Screening Test - Junior (DST-J). Harcourt Assessment, (2004)
  • [Fernández-Pinto et al., 2010] 4a ed, TEA, (2010)
  • [Goswami, 2011] A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15 (2011), pp. 3-10 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.001 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.001
  • [Goswami, 2018] A neural basis for phonological awareness? An oscillatory temporal-sampling perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27 (2018), pp. 56-63 https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417727520
  • [Goswami, 2019] Speech rhythm and language acquisition: An amplitude modulation phase hierarchy perspective. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1453 (2019), pp. 67-78 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14137 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14137
  • [Goswami y Leong, 2013] Speech rhythm and temporal structure: Converging perspectives?. Laboratory Phonology, 4 (2013), pp. 67-92 https://doi.org/10.1515/lp-2013-0004
  • [Goswami et al., 2013] Impaired perception of syllable stress in children with dyslexia: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 69 (2013), pp. 1-17 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.03.001
  • [Gutiérrez-Fresneda et al., 2021] Efectos de la conciencia suprasegmental en el aprendizaje de la lectura en los primeros cursos escolares. Revista de Psicodidáctica, 26 (2021), pp. 28-34 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psicod.2020.10.001
  • [Gutiérrez-Palma et al., 2016] Lexical stress awareness and orthographic stress in Spanish. Learning and Individual Differences, 45 (2016), pp. 144-150 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2015.11.026
  • [Gutiérrez-Palma y Palma-Reyes, 2007] Stress sensitivity and reading performance in Spanish: A study with children. Journal of Research in Reading, 30 (2007), pp. 157-168 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2007.00339.x
  • [Gutiérrez-Palma et al., 2019] Beyond phonological awareness: Stress awareness and learning word spelling. Learning and Individual Differences, 74. (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2019.101755
  • [Hayes, 2022] 3rd ed., The Guilford Press, (2022)
  • [Holliman et al., 2014] Modeling the relationship between prosodic sensitivity and early literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 49 (2014), pp. 469-482 https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.82
  • [Holliman et al., 2017] Examining the independent contribution of prosodic sensitivity to word reading and spelling in early readers. Reading and Writing, 30 (2017), pp. 509-521 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9687-z
  • [Holliman et al., 2008] Sensitivity to speech rhythm explains individual differences in reading ability independently of phonological awareness. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26 (2008), pp. 357-367 https://doi.org/10.1348/026151007X241623
  • [Jiménez-Fernández et al., 2015] Impaired stress awareness in Spanish children with developmental dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 37 (2015), pp. 152-161 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.11.002
  • [Kalashnikova et al., 2021] Rhythm discrimination and metronome tapping in 4-year-old children at risk for developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Development, 60 (2021), pp. 101129 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101129
  • [Kertész y Honbolygó, 2021] Tapping to music predicts literacy skills of first-grade children. Frontiers in Psychology, 12 (2021), pp. 741540 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.741540
  • [Kertész y Honbolygó, 2023] First school year tapping predicts children's third-grade literacy skills. Scientific Reports, 13 (2023), pp. 2298 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-29367-5 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-29367-5
  • [Lallier et al., 2018] From auditory rhythm processing to grapheme-to-phoneme conversion: How neural oscillations can shed light on developmental dyslexia. pp. 147-163 https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-90805-2_8
  • [Lê et al., 2020] Rhythm in the blood: The influence of rhythm skills on literacy development in third graders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 198 (2020), pp. 104880 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104880
  • [Lundetrae, 2015] Lundetræ, K. (2015). On track rhythm test (Version 1.0) [App]. https://itunes.apple.com/us/app/on-track-rhythm-test/id1063712637.
  • [Lundetrae y Thomson, 2018] Rhythm production at school entry as a predictor of poor reading and spelling at the end of first grade. Reading and Writing, 31 (2018), pp. 215-237 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9782-9 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9782-9
  • [Malhotra et al., 2017] 5th ed., Pearson Education, (2017)
  • [Mata y Serrano, 2019] EPALE. Test de Evaluación del Potencial de Aprendizaje para la Lecto-Escritura. Editorial Universidad de Granada, (2019)
  • [McAuley et al., 2006] The time of our lives: Life span development of timing and event tracking. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 135 (2006), pp. 348-367 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.348 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.348
  • [Melby-Lervag et al., 2012] Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138 (2012), pp. 322-352 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026744 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026744
  • [Moritz et al., 2013] Links between early rhythm skills, musical training, and phonological awareness. Reading and Writing, 26 (2013), pp. 739-769 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9389-0
  • [National Early Literacy Panel, 2008] National Early Literacy Panel (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. https://lincs.ed.gov/publications/pdf/NELPReport09.pdf.
  • [Ozernov-Palchik et al., 2018] Relationships between early literacy and nonlinguistic rhythmic processes in kindergarteners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 167 (2018), pp. 354-368 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.11.009 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.11.009
  • [Pérez-Navarro et al., 2022] Local temporal regularities in child-directed speech in Spanish. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 65 (2022), pp. 3776-3788 http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2022_JSLHR-22-00111 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1044/2022_JSLHR-22-00111
  • [Raven et al., 1996] Manual for Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices. Oxford Psychologists Press, (1996)
  • [Reifinger, 2006] Skill development in rhythm perception and performance: A review of literature. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 25 (2006), pp. 15-27 https://doi.org/10.1177/87551233060250010103
  • [Ríos-López et al., 2022] Right-hemisphere coherence to speech at pre-reading stages predicts reading performance one year later. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 34 (2022), pp. 179-193 https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2021.1986514
  • [Rivera Ibaceta y Moreira Tricet, 2020] Music as a promoter of phonological skills: An exploratory study with Uruguayan preschool children. Ciencias Psicológicas, 14 (2020), https://doi.org/10.22235/cp.v14i2.2270
  • [Seisdedos Cubero, 1996] 2ª ed, TEA, (1996)
  • [Tierney et al., 2021] Reading ability in children relates to rhythm perception across modalities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 210 (2021), pp. 105196 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105196 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105196
  • [Wood, 2006] Metrical stress sensitivity in young children and its relationship to phonological awareness and reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 29 (2006), pp. 270-287 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00308.x
  • [Woodruff Carr et al., 2014] Beat synchronization predicts neural speech encoding and reading readiness in preschoolers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111 (2014), pp. 14559-14564 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1406219111 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1406219111
  • [Zhang et al., 2021] Neurophysiological tracking of speech-structure learning in typical and dyslexic readers. Neuropsychologia, 158 (2021), pp. 107889 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107889 | Medline https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107889